New Study Overturns Long-Held Theory on Zebra Stripes


Anthropologist Amanda Melin leads research group’s finding that the patterns in a zebra’s hide don’t provide camouflage
Anthropologist Amanda Melin measures the luminance of zebra stripes at the Calgary Zoo using an LS-110 Minolta Spot Photometer. Photo by Tim Caro, UC Davis 
Anthropologist Amanda Melin measures the luminance of zebra stripes at the Calgary Zoo using an LS-110 Minolta Spot Photometer. Photo by Tim Caro, UC Davis

If you’ve always thought of a zebra’s stripes as offering some type of camouflaging protection against predators, it’s time to think again, suggest scientists at the University of Calgary and UC Davis.

“The most longstanding hypothesis for zebra striping is crypsis, or camouflaging, but until now the question has always been framed through human eyes,” said the study’s lead author Amanda Melin, an assistant professor of biological anthropology.

“We, instead, carried out a series of calculations through which we were able to estimate the distances at which lions and spotted hyenas, as well as zebras, can see zebra stripes under daylight, twilight, or during a moonless night.”

Melin conducted the study with Tim Caro, a UC Davis professor of wildlife biology, Donald Kline, a professor emeritus of psychology from the University of Calgary, whose research is focused on vision and perception, and Chihiro Hiramatsu of Kyushu University, Japan. Findings from their study were published Jan. 22 in the journal PLOS ONE.

In earlier studies, Caro and other colleagues have provided evidence suggesting that the zebra’s stripes provide an evolutionary advantage by discouraging biting flies, which are natural pests of zebras.

In the new study, Melin and her co-authors found that stripes cannot be involved in allowing the zebras to blend in with the background of their environment or in breaking up the outline of the zebra, because at the point at which predators can see a zebra’s stripes, they probably already have heard or smelled their zebra prey.

“The results from this new study provide no support at all for the idea that the zebra’s stripes provide some type of anti-predator camouflaging effect,” Caro said. “Instead, we reject this long-standing hypothesis that was debated by Charles Darwin and Alfred Russell Wallace.”

The zebra stripe riddle has puzzled scientists for over a century

To test the hypothesis that stripes camouflage zebras against the backdrop of their natural environment, the researchers passed digital images taken in the field in Tanzania through spatial and colour filters that simulated how the zebras would appear to their main predators — lions and spotted hyenas — as well as to other zebras.

They also measured the stripes’ widths and light contrast, or luminance, in order to estimate the maximum distance from which lions, spotted hyenas and zebras could detect stripes, using information about these animals’ visual capabilities.

They found that beyond 50 meters in daylight or 30 meters at twilight, when most predators hunt, stripes can be seen by humans but are hard for zebra predators to distinguish. And on moonless nights, the stripes are particularly difficult for all species to distinguish beyond nine meters. This suggests that the stripes don’t provide camouflage in woodland areas, where it had earlier been theorized that black stripes mimicked tree trunks and white stripes blended in with shafts of light through the trees.

And in open, treeless habitats, where zebras tend to spend most of their time, the researchers found that lions could see the outline of striped zebras just as easily as they could see similar-sized prey with fairly solid-colored hides, such as waterbuck and topi, and the smaller impala. It had been earlier suggested that the striping might disrupt the outline of zebras on the plains, where they might otherwise be clearly visible to their predators.

In addition to discrediting the camouflaging hypothesis, the study did not yield evidence suggesting that the striping provides some type of social advantage by allowing other zebras to recognize each other at a distance. While zebras can see stripes over somewhat further distances than their predators can, the researchers also noted that other species of animals that are closely related to the zebra are highly social and able to recognize other individuals of their species, despite having no striping to distinguish them.

Funding for the study was provided by the the National Sciences and Research Council of Canada, Wenner Gren Foundation, the National Geographic Society and UC Davis.

University of Calgary